Skip to main content
  • Annotated bibliography
  • Open access
  • Published:

Social capital and health: Does egalitarianism matter? A literature review

Abstract

The aim of the paper is to critically review the notion of social capital and review empirical literature on the association between social capital and health across countries. The methodology used for the review includes a systematic search on electronic databases for peer-reviewed published literature. We categorize studies according to level of analysis (single and multilevel) and examine whether studies reveal a significant health impact of individual and area level social capital. We compare the study conclusions according to the country's degrees of economic egalitarianism. Regardless of study design, our findings indicate that a positive association (fixed effect) exists between social capital and better health irrespective of countries degree of egalitarianism. However, we find that the between-area variance (random effect) in health tends to be lower in more egalitarian countries than in less egalitarian countries. Our tentative conclusion is that an association between social capital and health at the individual level is robust with respect to the degree of egalitarianism within a country. Area level or contextual social capital may be less salient in egalitarian countries in explaining health differences across places.

Introduction

The use of social factors to explain community health status is not a new phenomenon. Since Durkheim's classic work on suicide, the importance of social integration and social capital has been recognised for population well-being [1]. Nonetheless, the notion of 'social capital' has attracted wide-ranging attention in the social sciences and public health literature over the last decade [e.g., [2–10]]. In the public health arena, the concept attracted attention following the work of James Coleman and Robert Putnam [11, 12]. A growing body of empirical research has been conducted on the links between individual (micro) and area (macro/meso) level social capital and population health [e.g. [13–16]].

Social capital is a multifaceted phenomenon [2]. Among other views, social capital can be considered as a by-product of social relationships resulting from reciprocal exchanges between members involved in social associations or networks and can be recognized as a public good that generates positive externalities facilitating cooperation for the achievement of common goals [17]. It is thought that social capital may generate material/market and non-material/non-market returns to the individual. Material return may include higher wage, better employment prospects or reduced transaction costs, while non-market returns may include improvements in the quality of the individual's relationships and improvements in health or even happiness [3]. It is also hypothesised that there are both direct and indirect returns on the production and accumulation of health and social capital. Direct returns stem from the fact that both health and social capital enhance individual welfare, while indirect returns come about as a result of the observation that health capital increases the amount of productive time, and social capital improves the efficiency of the production technology used for producing health capital [18].

Whilst 'egalitarianism' is a multifaceted notion in social and political thought, common forms of egalitarianism include economic egalitarianism, moral egalitarianism, legal egalitarianism, democratic egalitarianism, political egalitarianism, gender egalitarianism and opportunity egalitarianism [19]. In modern democratic societies, the term "egalitarian" is often used to refer to a situation that favors (for any of a wide range of reasons), a greater degree of equality of income and wealth across a population. Economic egalitarianism, (popular with liberals throughout much of the 20th Century), has given way to a concern not that everyone be strictly equal in material possession, but rather that everyone be equal in having enough material goods to effectively fulfill his or her native human capacities [19].

In the present paper the notion of egalitarianism is considered from material aspects and operationalized on the basis of a country's overall level of income inequality and total public social expenditure as a percentage of GDP. In a recent Luxembourg Income Study (LIS) report, Mahler & Jesuit [20] note that the Gini-index of private sector income inequality in the reputed egalitarian countries such as Sweden (44.1) and the Netherlands (45.8) are almost at the same level as in the USA (44.7) or the UK (47.5). However, the authors point out that the egalitarian distribution of disposable income (post-government income) through government transfers and taxes in Sweden and the Netherlands make the difference with the USA or the UK. Therefore, as a marker of income inequality we consider Gini-coefficients based on disposable income rather than private income. Despite this clarification, there may still remain some difficulties in classifying countries by their degree of egalitarianism. However, for the sake of simplicity we consider a country with a comparatively lower Gini-coefficient based on disposable income and a higher share of public social expenditure to be more egalitarian than a country with the reverse case. Based on country-level Gini-coefficients (based on disposable income) and share of public social expenditure in percentage of GDP, we classified countries as 'egalitarian', 'moderately egalitarian' and 'not egalitarian' as provided in Table 1[21–23]. Still, there may remain some cautions. Due to the fact that the UK is a country with a relatively higher Gini-coefficient and higher public social expenditure compared with (in our classification) moderately egalitarian countries, its position may be seen as tentative in this study. Although it may be seen as somewhat arbitrary to categorize countries as egalitarian or not, for the sake of parsimony we define Nordic countries (namely, Finland, Norway and Sweden), Germany and the Netherlands as relatively more egalitarian compared to other countries.

Table 1 Classification of countries by income distribution and public social expenditure, and summary of results

Why would a more egalitarian distribution of income and wealth matter to social capital and its impact on health? As a complementary question, one could inquire whether egalitarianism makes any difference in the formation of social capital in a society. In trying to respond to this query researchers have been puzzled by an apparently paradoxical coexistence of a greater stock of social capital and an egalitarian distribution of income and wealth through extensive welfare-state arrangements such as seen in the Scandinavian countries [24]. For instance, Alan Wolfe argued that in societies where citizens are protected "from cradle to grave" by the state, civil society and norms of reciprocity or social capital are "crowded out" [[25], p.142, also cited in [24]]. However, in reality this is not the case. Empirical research shows that in contrast to the trends described in the USA [12] there is little evidence of a decline in social capital in Sweden. Rather, in terms of trust, associational membership and informal interaction, social capital seems to have been maintained at comparatively high levels in Scandinavia [26]. For instance, the World Values Surveys in 1981, 1991–92 and 1995–96 show that 60% of the population in Sweden (second highest, 61% in Norway) agree with the statement that 'most other people can be trusted'; the percentage for the USA is 49 [27]. Recently, Kumlin and Rothstein tried to resolve this paradox empirically using Swedish data. Their findings indicate that welfare-state institutions have a capacity for both making and breaking social capital in Sweden [24]. They show that contacts with universal welfare-state institutions tend to increase social trust; by contrast, means-tested programmes tend to diminish trust.

The next issue to resolve is whether greater (lower) intensity of material egalitarianism can modify the health impact of social capital in a society. It is documented that an egalitarian distribution of wealth and income appears to imply a more cohesive, harmonious society and the level of income inequality may affect both a society's cohesiveness and its members' health [28]. By examining the relationships between income inequality, social capital and health, Wilkinson has argued that the social environment is more cohesive in more egalitarian societies; the members of these societies tend to be less violent, less prone to commit homicide, less hostile to one another, and trust other members more [29, 30]. Using data from 39 US states, Kawachi and colleagues demonstrated that income inequality is positively associated with disinvestment in social capital, which is in turn linked with increased mortality [17]. Kawachi et al. have also shown a strong correlation between trust and income inequality in the United States, while income distribution in states was linked to variations in mortality and, indeed, death from specific causes such as heart disease, cancer and homicide [17].

Within a defined economic and political system (such as a country), the effect of different forms of social capital on health may also be conditioned by the country's intensity of economic egalitarianism. More specifically, it is arguable whether findings from countries such as the USA are generalizable to other countries which are relatively more egalitarian. Besides, unlike the conclusions of most research conducted in the USA, it has been observed that in Canada and Sweden there are no relationships between income inequality and mortality, neither at the level of provinces nor in metropolitan areas [31]. In the Swedish context, Gerdtham and Johannesson examined whether mortality is related to individual income, mean community income, and community income inequality [32]. They concluded that municipality level income inequality had no effect on mortality. At the county level, they found some indications of an association between income inequality and mortality; however, the counties which were more unequal had lower mortality.

Different public social institutions within welfare-states and more equitable distribution of income and wealth in the countries may modify the health impact of social capital or the effect of different forms of social capital (e.g. individual level and area level). Area level social capital (either derived from individual responses or contextual in itself) may not be relevant or important to public health if the societal level of equity or public safety net provision ensures resources for the individual. Given societal variations in economic egalitarianism, it is interesting to consider whether the effect of social capital on health varies among geographical areas according to the degree of equity and safety net provision within countries as well.

The aim of this paper is three-fold. Firstly, we critically review the origins and different forms and dimensions of social capital as it has been operationalized in the empirical literature. Secondly, we systematically review the empirical studies that have examined the health impact of individual and area level social capital for different countries by surveying both single level and multilevel studies. Thirdly, we explore some analytical and interpretational issues that may be pertinent when assessing the health impact of area level social capital. In section 4 we interpret our review results according to both single and multilevel studies and also by country, as well as by fixed and random effect results from multilevel studies. The paper ends with a discussion and conclusions along with some suggestions on directions for future research.

Origins and definitions of social capital

(This section is mainly based on Portes [33] and Woolcock [34]).

In the literature there is no consensus on the intellectual origins or who first implicitly or explicitly introduced the notion of 'social capital'. Different authors with diverse background trace the idea from different intellectual origins. In particular, American sociologist A. Portes argues that the idea is imported from the 19th-century foundations of sociology and he recognizes that the concept is implicit in the works of Emile Durkheim and Karl Marx [33, 35]. He further claims that Pierre Bourdieu [36] was the first to systematically and explicitly analyze the notion of social capital in the present sense. Bourdieu defines social capital as "the aggregate of the actual or potential resources which are linked to possession of a durable network of more or less institutionalized relationships of mutual acquaintance or recognition" [[37], p.248].

After reviewing the intellectual history of social capital, Woolcock notes that though employed for different purposes, the word 'social capital' was used by two major proponents of economic sciences, Marshall and Hicks, 'to distinguish between temporary and permanent stocks of physical capital' [cited in [34], p 159]. He also acknowledges the works of David Hume and Edmund Burke as intellectual origins of the notion [[34] & also cited in [35]]. Refereeing to Swedberg [38], Woolcock further claims "the Durkheimian, Weberian and Marxist traditions within classical sociology were all heavily influenced by the economic debates and issues of that period, and much of what we now refer to as 'social capital' lay at heart of these concerns" [[34] p.160]. As an explicit pioneer of the notion, Woolcock gives credit to Lyda Hanifan (in discussing rural school community centers, see [39, 40] – also cited in [34], endnote 13]. Hanifan uses the term to illustrate "those tangible substances [that] count for most in the daily lives of people" ([40] p. 130). However, both Putnam [41] and Woolcock [34] refer to Jane Jacobs [42] for the first use of the term 'social capital' in its contemporary sense.

In a recent review work, Durlauf & Fafchamps [43] assert that the concept is first introduced into modern social science research by economist Glen Loury [44] who defined social capital as "naturally occurring social relationships among persons which promote or assist the acquisition of skills and traits valued in the marketplace an asset which may be as significant as financial bequests in accounting for the maintenance of inequality of our society" [[44], p.100].

Whilst it is not unambiguous who first implicitly or explicitly introduced the notion of social capital in its present sense, it is however clear that after Loury and Bourdieu, the concept is further developed, modified and disseminated in the diverse disciplines by the works of Coleman [11], Putnam et al. [45] and Portes [33]. The definitions given by these proponents of the notion are cited as follows:

Coleman: "consist of some aspect of social structure and they facilitate certain actions of individuals who are within the structure" [[11], p.302].

Putnam et al: "refers to features of social organization, such as trust, norms and networks that can improve the efficiency of society by facilitating coordinated actions" [[45], p.167].

Portes: "refers to the capacity of individuals to command scare resources by virtue of their membership in networks or broader social structure" [[33], p.12].

Although overlapping to some extent, four main theoretical ingredients can be identified in the definition of social capital: social trust/reciprocity, collective efficacy, participation in voluntary organizations and social integration for mutual benefit [9]. After surveying different empirical studies and definitions of social capital, Durlauf & Fafchamps distinguish three main basic ideas:"(a) social capital generates positive externalities for members of a group; (b) these externalities are achieved thanks to shared trust, norms and values and their effects on expectations and behavior; and (c) shared trust, norms and values arise from informal forms of organizations based on social network and association. The study of social capital is that of network-based process that generate beneficial outcomes through norms and trust". [[43], p.5].

The forms and dimensions of social capital

As seen in Figure 1, social capital can be broken down into cognitive and structural components [46–48]. Cognitive social capital includes norms, values, attitudes and beliefs. Structural components of social capital refer to externally observable aspects of social organization, such as the density of social networks, or patterns of civic engagement. The structural components of social capital are contextual in its nature. Structural and cognitive social capital are complementary. The cognitive component assesses people's perceptions of the level of interpersonal trust, sharing, and reciprocity. The structural component of social capital examines the extent and intensity of associational links and activity in society such as measures of informal sociability, density of civic associations, and indicators of civic engagement [47]. In relation to health, cognitive social capital (predominantly captured at the micro level) is believed to shape behavioral norms, through control of risk behavior, provision of mutual aid and support, and informal means of informational exchange [49]. Structural social capital at the macro level is shaped by institutions, policies, and culture. Different forms and dimensions of social capital along with their operationalization are also specified in figure 1.

Figure 1
figure 1

Forms and dimensions of social capital with operationalization of the notion in empirical studies.

As noticed in Figure 1, two distinct types of social capital are recognized – horizontal, reflecting ties that exist among individuals or groups of equals or near-equals, and vertical (also referred to as linking social capital), stemming from hierarchical or unequal relations due to differences in power or resource bases and status [50]. Additional distinctions have been drawn within horizontal social capital, viz., "bonding" social capital (also called localized social capital) and "bridging" social capital [12, 51]. Bonding social capital refers to the relations within homogenous groups. In other words, these are the strong ties that connect family members, neighbors, and close friends and colleagues. By contrast, bridging social capital is heterogeneous by definition. The weak ties that link those of different ethnic and occupational backgrounds form "bridging" social capital, including formal or informal social interactions [12, 50–53].

Bonding relationships act as the primary means for the transmission of behavioral norms to family members and friends. Bonding social capital is important for establishing and favoring healthy norms, controlling abnormal social behavior and for generating mutual aid, and protecting the vulnerable [49]. By contrast, bridging social capital is important to the success of civil society and it is also recognised as an important source of other benefits for individuals, communities, and societies. It offers members of the society opportunities for participation in heterogeneous groups of people from diverse social classes and opens channels to voice concern in favor of those who may have very little opportunity to reach more formal avenues in order to affect societal changes, e.g. change in public welfare-oriented policies [49]. Virtually no studies have explicitly measured and tested the bridging form of social capital and its relationship to health. Theoretically, bridging social capital may be associated with better health because it enables disadvantaged groups to access material resources through connections to socially advantaged groups. However, in one of the few published studies to measure bridging social capital, Mitchell and LaGory found bonding social capital was associated with worse mental health in a disadvantaged black community in the USA, whereas bridging social capital was associated with better mental health [54]. Bridging social capital may also be critical for the prevention of inter-ethnic and religious conflict and violence [55].

Critiques of social capital

A common criticism of the concept of social capital concerns measurement. The meaning of the concept continues to be contested, and consensus about its measurement remains elusive. Baum finds that the current concepts of social capital are "vague, slippery, and poorly specified, and in danger of 'meaning all things to all people' on both the right and left of the political spectrum" [56]. Gillies depicts social capital as "a descriptive construct rather than an explanatory theory" [[57] & cited in [58]]. Like other social scientists, economists have also criticized the definitions of social capital, pointing out the vagueness and inconsistency of various definitions [which may be found in e.g. [2, 43, 59] &[60]].

There are also some ambiguities concerning where exactly social capital resides. One important distinction is between social capital as an individual attribute and social capital as a collective characteristic [10, 35]. Almost all approaches to the measurement (or conceptualization) of social capital confuse the differences between social capital as a social resource, as a social product, or as an individual response [35]. With the exception of Portes, this problem is inherent in the work of all major proponents of the concept of social capital. By contrast, Portes has clearly described social capital as an individual attribute [33, 35].

Moreover, the operationalization and measurement of social capital remains a challenging task to researchers. From the perspective of empirical research, quantifying social capital involves identifying observable variables that can be used as proxies for social capital [33]. Additionally, it is also recognized that indicators such as 'norms' and 'shared values' are notoriously difficult to measure [43]. Furthermore, indicators of social capital are not routinely available on administrative data sets (such as the government census). Even when special surveys have been collected to measure social capital, the relevant spatial scale for operationalizing the concept (the neighborhood versus the state, or even entire societies) presents an analytical dilemma [9, 35, 61, 62]. The Modifiable Area Unit Problem (MAUP) has been acknowledged as a potentially worrying feature of aggregated data. MAUP is a result of the imposition of artificial units of spatial aggregation on a continuous geographical phenomenon, resulting in the construction of an artificial spatial pattern [63, 64]. MAUP is not unique to the analysis of social capital and has also been found in the analysis of area-based socioeconomic and epidemiological data [65, 66].

Social capital is not a panacea for population health problems and it may not always generate better health outcomes. Researchers in social capital have been criticized for failing to consider such negative outcomes. Social capital can sometimes facilitate negative or perverse consequences. Portes acknowledges four instances of negative consequences: exclusion of outsiders from resources controlled by network members, excess claims made on successful members by free-riding fellow members, restrictions on individual freedoms (particularly in closely bonded networks), and the downward leveling of norms, which may block members of historically oppressed groups from participation in mainstream society [33]. Similarly, Baum recognizes that interconnection or close-knit association may not necessarily be 'healthy', particularly for outsiders [56]. Muntaner et al. also point out that strong associations among individuals may both increase and decrease the risk of certain health outcomes [67]. For example, strong friendship networks of peers may increase the risks of smoking, drinking, or use of illicit drugs, while in different circumstances these same sorts of connections may decrease the risk of suicide. In the Swedish context, using cross-sectional survey data, Lundborg examines school class-based peer effects in binge drinking, smoking and illicit drug use and finds significant positive peer effects for all three activities [68].

Finally, the literature on social capital and health has also been criticized by neo-materialists who emphasize the importance of political regimes, ideology, and institutions for good health both at the population and individual level [69]. The neo-materialists have even accused social capital theorists of "blaming the community" for their problems, such as poor health outcomes [69, 70]. Navarro criticizes the social capital literature for exaggerating its importance for health. He also points out that class relations are absent from social epidemiology and public health research – class relations may be a more important determinant of population health than social capital [71].

Methods

Search strategy and inclusion criteria

The methodology used was a systematic search on electronic databases for published literature. A detailed search of the databases was conducted for articles published between January 1st 1995 and June 15th, 2005: MEDLINE (via Pub Med), Sociological Abstracts (via CSA), EconLit (via CSA), and International Bibliography of the Social Sciences, IBSS, (via CSA). The review used the following key words: "social capital" AND "health" OR "mortality" OR "self-rated health" OR "morbidity".

We mainly included the social capital studies that consider direct health status measures and mortality as outcome variable(s) and studies that used a quantitative methodology and were published in peer reviewed journals. Once the searches were completed, the title, key words, and abstracts were reviewed for final selection. Unpublished data were excluded from these analyses, as well as articles published in languages other than English. In total, over 653 articles were initially identified as potentially fitting the selection criteria (233 from Medline and the rest from other data bases, i.e. EconLit (103), Sociological Abstracts (229) and IBSS (88). From the initial searches, articles were excluded where the title and abstract made it clear that the paper did not fulfill the inclusion criteria. Collectively, these search strategies resulted in a total of 42 articles fitting the study inclusion criteria.

Classification of studies

Macinko and Starfield state that social capital can be analyzed at four different levels: the macro level (countries, regions and counties), the meso level (neighborhoods), the micro level (social networks of individuals) as well as the individual attitudinal/psychological level (trust) [35]. Furthermore, depending on analytical approach and study design, health impact of social capital studies can be classified by the level of analysis and unit of analysis. With regard to the level, a study may be a single level or a multilevel study. To differentiate between single level and multilevel studies, we indicate whether studies use an analytical approach that explicitly recognizes a hierarchical structure for the data.

Depending on the study design (unit of analysis), single level studies can be further distinguished as individual level studies and ecological studies. In individual studies, the unit of observation and analysis is the subjects (all variables, e.g. response variable – health status and predictor – social capital, are gathered as individual attributes). In ecological studies the unit of analysis is a group of individuals who are clustered together according to geo-demographic, socioeconomic, or other criteria (e.g. state/municipality/neighborhood as unit of analysis) [72]. In the ecological studies both health status and social capital are examined at the aggregate level.

Multilevel analysis studies are concerned with analyzing data with a nested structure. The main reason for employing multilevel statistical analysis is because we are interested in explicitly modeling the contextual heterogeneity. In multilevel analysis, the term 'contextual effects' is generally used to refer to the effects of variables (e.g. social capital) at a higher level (state/community/neighborhood) on outcomes defined at a lower level (e.g. individual level health status)[73]. In multilevel analysis, one may also control for individual level social capital, when analyzing area level social capital variables or 'contextual social capital'. Also, the term 'contextual social capital' refer occasionally to the effects of area level social capital variables and may be classified as a 'derived variable' – summarizing specific characteristics of individuals within the group or area (e.g. overall civic participation or level of trust) – and 'integral variable'- no individual level analogues but rather a group or area level construct (e.g. number of voluntary organizations/political parties in an area, crime rate etc) [73].

To avoid the confusion of trying to identify the different kinds of social capital used in the reviewed studies, we categorize social capital into three groups, irrespective of different study design: individual level social capital, 'aggregated' level social capital (individual social capital but aggregated to area level or derived as an area level social capital) and contextual social capital (integral social capital variable).

Results

A total of 42 papers were reviewed. These papers come mostly from OECD countries, representing a total of 30 single-level studies (17 individual level and 13 ecological studies) and 12 multilevel studies. These comprised of 13 papers (6 multilevel studies) using US data, 4 studies (1 multilevel) from Canada, 4 from eastern Europe (no multilevel study), 4 from western Europe (2 multilevel studies), 7 from Scandinavia (1 multilevel), 3 from Australia (no multilevel study), and 7 cross-country studies (1 multilevel). A concise summary of the studies on the associations between social capital and health status (mortality or self-rated health/health) by different countries is presented in Table 1. Tables 2a and 2b summarize the sources and characteristics of the studies, i.e. (a) study design (unit of analysis- whether ecological or individual based analysis, data etc), (b) conceptualization and operationalization of the social capital variable (how social capital is defined and measured and kind of measurement), and (c) outcome variable(s) (which response variable(s) was considered for the studies) along with (d) their main findings. In the last column of Table 2a, countries are classified according to their degree of egalitarianism.

Table 2 Empirical evidence by types of studies and countries: Single level studies
Table 3 Empirical evidence by types of studies and countries: Multilevel studies

Single level studies (individual and ecological)

Studies in North America (USA & Canada)

Studies of social capital and health in the USA have found an association between a variety of indicators (trust, reciprocity, group membership) and health outcomes (lower all-cause and cause-specific mortality and better self-rated health) [17, 74–78]. Ecological studies in the USA have primarily been carried out at the state level, with one exception [75] involving neighborhoods in the city of Chicago. In studying the effects of social capital all these studies used 'aggregated' level social capital. For instance, using three US General Social Survey (GSS) measures of social capital (individuals responses on the items such as, social distrust, perceived lack of fairness and helpfulness of others), Kawachi et al. reported that each of the measures of 'aggregated' social capital was associated with income inequality and mortality in the USA states [16]. Wilkinson et al. found that social mistrust is closely associated with mortality and violent crime rates in 39 US states [74].

Another state level ecological analysis was conducted by Milyo and Mellor [77]. They considered both crude and age-adjusted mortality rates as health measures and both contextual and 'aggregated' social capital were measured by Robert Putnam's index of state social capital and by an index of social mistrust derived from responses to the General Social Survey. However, the study did not find significant associations between mortality and either income inequality, minority racial concentration, or social capital. They concluded that different age-adjustment methods can cause a change in the sign or statistical significance of the association between mortality and other socioeconomic factors.

In Canada, Veenstra's findings for Saskatchewan are also different from most of the US studies. Veenstra found that individual level social capital (overall civic participation, trust in government, trust in neighborhood, etc) was not significantly related to self-rated health among the general population but had a positive impact among the elderly [79]. In another study, he showed that individual level social capital (associational density and civic participation) was inversely and weakly associated with age-adjusted mortality rates but not with low-birth weight rate [80]. In a recent study, Veenstra et al. (2005) found that individual level social capital (measured by constructing an index focusing specifically on breadth and depth of involvement in voluntary associations) particularly, overall involvement in voluntary associations was significantly associated with emotional distress and body-mass index and marginally related with self-rated health before and after controlling for age, gender, and neighborhood of residence [81]. They concluded that more participation in voluntary associations apparently had a positive association with well-being operationalized by these health measures.

Studies in eastern Europe

In Russia, Kennedy et al. showed that 'aggregated' social capital indicators (social cohesion, civic engagement and trust in government) were strongly associated with lower mortality rates during the period of the Russian mortality crisis [8]. Rose found that both human capital and individual level social capital measures (sense of self-efficacy, trust of others, informal networks and social support) were associated with better self-rated health in Russia [82]. Skrabski and colleagues also found associations between 'aggregated' social capital variables (lack of social trust, reciprocity between citizens and help received from civil organizations) measured at the county level in Hungary and rates of middle age mortality and all cause mortality rates [83, 84].

Studies in western Europe

Using the British Household Panel Study for the years 1998 and 1999, McCulloch found that lower individual level social capital (social disorganization) was associated with increased risk of psychiatric morbidity [85].

Considering vertical contextual social capital (party political affiliation and general election voting pattern was used to measure social capital) in Ireland, Kelleher et al. found that there was no significant relation between Standardized Mortality Ratios (SMR) and voting pattern for the two main political parties but a significant relation with left wing voting [86]. There was a significant positive relation between left wing voting and dissatisfaction with health as measured from selected reported measures of health status and lifestyle.

Studies in Scandinavia

In Finland, Hyyppä and Mäki compared active life expectancies and disability pensions between two neighboring regions consisting of both bilingual (half Swedish and half Finnish) and entirely Finnish-speaking municipalities [87]. They found that the individuals belonging to the Swedish speaking community remained active in working life longer and had a significantly longer life span than their Finnish speaking counterparts in the same region. They suggested that the observed inequalities in active life and in mortality depend on differences in the extent of individual level social capital. In another study Hyyppä and Mäki found that the Swedish speaking community seemed to have access to a greater stock of individual level social capital than their Finnish speaking counterparts, which in turn was significantly and positively associated with better self-rated health [88]. Using the same data set, Hyyppä and Mäki recently examined which social capital indicators were important for health and sought to identify possible pathways through which social capital might influence individual health [88]. After controlling for health-related behavior, the results showed that Finnish speakers were more likely to be migrants, to mistrust others, and to be less active in community events. They also concluded that active participation in voluntary associations and friendship ties are associated with better self-rated health in a bilingual community.

Using a prospective cohort study design among public sector employees in Finland, Liukkonen and colleagues found that a low level of individual level 'workplace social capital' constructed from information on employment security and social support/co-worker support and combining them into a variable indicating the amount of 'social job capital'/social capital was associated with poor health (measured by a 5-point scale of self-rated health and psychological distress) [90]. However, in the age-adjusted model this significant association remained only for women. After accounting for baseline health differences and other background variables, any significant associations for either women or men disappeared.

Employing a set of individual panel data from Statistics Sweden's Survey of Living Conditions (the ULF survey), Bolin et al. reported that individual level social capital (i.e. having a close friend outside of the household) had a positive effect on self-rated health [18]. Using a cross-sectional follow-up study based on data (from 1990 and 1991) from the Swedish Annual Level-of-Living Survey (SALLS), Sundquist et al. concluded that persons with low social participation in the social participation index had an increased risk of CHD [91].

More recently, employing cross-sectional data from Scania, the southernmost region of Sweden, Lindström constructed a social capital indicator. Based on both individual social participation in society and individual perception of generalised trust of other people combined with social participation and trust, the indicator yielded four alternative social capital categories: high-social participation/high trust (high social capital), high-social participation/low trust ("miniaturized community"), low-social participation/high trust (traditionalism), and low-social participation/low trust (low social capital) [92]. The author found that for both males and females low trust was significantly associated with higher probability of poor self-reported global health. The odds ratios of poor self-reported health were significantly higher in the categories of high-social participation/low trust ("miniaturized community"), low-social participation/high trust ("traditionalism") and low-social participation/low trust (low-social capital) compared to the high-social capital (high-social participation/high trust) category among both men and women. The highest probability of poor self-reported global health was observed in the low-social capital categories in both sexes.

Studies in Australia

Using state level data, collected from several Australian Bureau of Statistics documents for the years 1990–1996, and five different indicators of social integration (percentage of people living alone, divorce rate, unemployment rate, proportion of people who are discouraged job-seekers and unionization rate) as a proxy for contextual social capital, Siahpush and Singh found that higher levels of most of the indicators of social capital were significantly associated with lower state level different cause-specific mortality, all cause mortality and sex specific life expectancy in Australia [93].

However, employing household level cross-sectional data among two disadvantaged neighborhoods in south-western Sydney and using six common individual level social capital components (neighborhood attachment, support networks, feelings of trust and reciprocity, local engagement, personal attachment to the area, feelings about safety and pro-activity in the social context), Chavez et al. concluded that, with the exception of feelings of trust and reciprocity, no other social capital component made significant contributions to explain self-rated health variance and that macro-level factors such as housing conditions and employment opportunities emerged as key explanatory factors[94].

Similar results were also found by Ziersch et al. in the Western suburbs of Adelaide, Australia considering respondent's physical health as dependent variable and individual level social capital as operationalized by neighborhood connections, neighborhood trust, reciprocity, neighborhood safety, and local civic action [95]. They showed that only perceived neighborhood safety was related to physical health and neighborhood connections and neighborhood safety were positively associated with mental health, proving subjects with stronger neighborhood connections and higher levels of perceived neighborhood safety to have better mental health.

Cross-country studies

Using World Values Survey (1990–1991) data for 16 OECD countries, Lynch et al. observed weak and inconsistent associations between a country's 'aggregated' level of social capital (social distrust, organization membership and volunteering) and age-specific and cause specific mortality rates [96].

Employing panel data from 19 OECD countries, Kennelly and colleagues also found little statistically significant positive associations between 'aggregated' level social capital (general trust and membership in voluntary organization) and population health as measured by three indicators of health status – life expectancy at birth, infant mortality and perinatal mortality [97].

Using data from the World Values Survey, 1995–97, Smith and Polanyi also found that individual level social capital (socially oriented behaviors and existence of socially oriented norms or behaviors) did not reduce the likelihood of lower income groups reporting poor self-rated health, relative to the highest income groups [98].

Employing World Value Survey data (for the years 1995–97) for 18 European countries, another cross-country study was done by Carlson where social capital was measured from an individual perspective, conceptualized as the degree of the individual's trust in people, confidence in the legal system, or membership of organizations [99]. The study found that both economic factors and some aspects of individual level social capital play a role in the area differences in self-rated health. However, economic factors appeared to be more important. People in the countries in central and eastern Europe tend to be worse off than in western Europe, both in terms of economy and in terms of social capital.

Helliwell and Putnam used large samples of data from three different sources, namely the World Values Survey, a three-wave panel for the years 1980, 1991–1992 and 1995–1997 covering 49 countries, the Social Capital Benchmark Survey in the USA, and Canadian data drawn from two national waves and two special over-samples of a survey. They found that individual level social capital (measured by indicators such as the strength of family, neighborhood, religious and community ties) was strongly associated with subjective well-being through many independent channels and in several different forms. They concluded that all indicators of social capital appeared independently and robustly related to happiness and life satisfaction, both directly and through their impact on health [100].

A cross-national study based data on Germany and the USA was done by Pollack and Knesebeck, where individual level social capital indicators included both norms (reciprocity and civic trust) and behaviors (social participation) [101]. The study showed that lack of reciprocity was associated with poorer self-rated health in both countries. Civic mistrust was associated with poorer self-rated health in both countries and with functional limitations and depression in the USA but not in Germany. Lack of participation was found to be associated with poorer self-rated health and depression in Germany and the effect of norms was found to be stronger in the USA than in Germany.

Multilevel studies

Multilevel regression analysis allows us to identify and quantify the extent to which variations in health are attributable to the characteristics of the area in which the individual resides. In particular, this technique enables us to determine if area level social capital affects individual health over and above, or in interaction with, individual characteristics. Multilevel regression analysis allows researchers to investigate how much of the area differences in health can be explained by differences in the individual composition of an area, and how much of these area differences are explained by their level of social capital. Among other reasons for applying multilevel regression techniques is (using statistical terminology) the existence of an area conditioned residual correlation. Failure to consider this dependence produces an underestimation of the standard errors of the regression coefficients [102, 103].

Multilevel analysis, initially applied in the areas of sociology, education, and demography, has recently attracted increased attention in the field of public health [73]. Researchers are also bringing to light the role of contextual and environmental factors on health [104–106]. There are two complementary approaches to understanding contextual effects (either based on derived or integrated variables) on individual health [104]. One approach – the analysis of traditional measures of association or fixed effect results – focuses on understanding how area characteristics are associated to individual health over and above individual factors. The other approach – the analysis of measures of health variation or random effect results – focuses on how health outcomes are distributed within and between different levels. Both approaches deserve to be investigated since they provide relevant and complementary information. In the present paper we summarize both the fixed effect results and random effect results of published multi-level studies, as this is a key to understanding the interpretation made by the authors. Table 2b provides a summary of the recent multilevel studies on social capital and health.

Fixed effect results

Studies in North America (USA & Canada)

Multilevel studies based on data from the USA have generally demonstrated a significant fixed effect of area level social capital on population health. With the exception of one, all studies used 'aggregated' social capital at the area level [see, [107–111]]. After adjusting for individual compositional variations, these studies found that living in an area with higher level social capital (measured by the indicators such as civic trust, reciprocity and civic engagement, volunteering, mistrust, interpersonal trust degree of trust worthiness of neighbors, density of local networking, and social cohesion) were strongly associated (fixed effects) with individual well-being and health [7, 14, 107–111].

For instance, Sampson et al. observed that an index including mutual trust and social control (social capital/social cohesion) is significantly inversely associated with neighborhood violence and homicide rates for the neighborhoods in Chicago [107]. Employing the 2000 Social Capital Community Benchmark Survey, Subramanian et al. examined the compositional and contextual effects of social trust on individual self-rated health across 40 US communities [14]. After controlling for individual demographic and socioeconomic status variables, the authors found an association between community levels of social trust (derived from individual level responses) and self-rated health. However, this association disappeared after controlling for individual trust perception in the model, although a significant cross-level interaction effect was found between community and individual trust. That is, self-reported health was lowest among individuals who expressed low trust and lived in communities with higher levels of trust.

Employing information on premature mortality from heart disease, Franzini and Spears found that although individual level characteristics were major predictors of premature heart disease mortality, social context/contextual social capital (operationalized by homeownership, i.e. percent of owner-occupied housing units at the tract and county level, and the crime index, defined as number of serious crimes known to the police per 100,000 population at the county level) at the block-group, tract, and county level also played an important role in explaining years of life lost to heart disease [111]. The study concluded that block-group level wealth, tract level own group ethnic density, and county level social capital had significant effects on years of life lost to heart disease in Texas.

A recent cross-sectional multilevel study was conducted by Veenstra where self-rated health (including both physical and mental health) was considered as a response variable and social capital was measured at both individual and community level [112]. Individual-level social capital was operationalized through individuals' perceptions about social and political trust and participation in voluntary associations and also measured attributes of communities such as (a) the number of public spaces per capita (sports, recreational, casual and social, cultural, religious, school, and hall spaces in particular), (b) the number of voluntary organizations per capita (sports and athletics, community, minorities, arts and culture, business, political, health and social services, religious and other organizations in particular), and (c) average levels of community and political trust (aggregates of the trust scales). The study found that the strongest predictors of fair/poor health were age, political trust and income, while community level variables were not significantly related to self-rated health. The study further concluded that household income and individual level political trust were particularly important predictors of long-term illness, but contextual social capital (at the community level) was mostly irrelevant in this instance.

Studies in western Europe

Using a longitudinal cohort study of 7236 children and their families in the city of Maastricht, the Netherlands, Drukker et al. found that 'aggregated' level social capital within neighborhoods was non-specifically associated with children's general health and satisfaction [113]. The mental health and behavior dimensions were more specifically associated with the degree of informal social control in the neighborhood.

In a recent study employing the Health and Lifestyle Survey (HALS), a follow-up study based on an English sample (from 1984/85 to 2001) and using different 'aggregated' level social capital indicators, Mohan et al. find little support for social capital as an area influence on the probability of survival [114].

Studies in Scandinavia

A significant fixed effect of social capital on self-rated health has also been observed in a Swedish study conducted by Lindström and colleagues. They assessed the influence of neighborhood level social participation and individual factors on self-reported health in the city of Malmö and concluded that 'aggregated' level social capital is significantly associated with individual self-reported health [15].

Cross-country studies

A recent study by Drukker et al. used data from the Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods (PHDCN), USA and the Maastricht Quality of Life study (MQoL) [115]. Subjective neighborhood social capital was used and operationalized through individual perceptions about informal social control (ISC) and social cohesion and trust (SC&T). Children's (age 11–12) perceived health was measured using a 5-item Likert type scale. Their findings indicated that Chicago had lower levels of SC&T while Maastricht had lower levels of ISC. The differences in both ISC and SC&T between the two cities are approximately half a standard deviation. Moreover, higher levels of 'aggregated' ISC and 'aggregated' SC&T were associated with higher levels of children's perceived health, in both Maastricht and the Chicago Hispanic sub-sample, but not in the Chicago non-Hispanic samples.

Random effect results

In investigating contextual/area determinants of health, the multilevel modelling approach allows for partitioning of variation arising at different levels of the hierarchy (e.g. individual and area), and explicit modelling of this variation allows for such insights to be made [116]. To assess the extent to which contextual effects of areas play a role in determining individual health or health behavior, the variance partition coefficient (VPC) or intra-cluster correlation (ICC) statistics provides an approach for identifying and quantifying area influences on population health. However, it is evident from our review that most studies do not report random part results for both higher and lower level variances, nor do they typically report ICCs.

Studies in North America (USA & Canada)

Random part results not being reported in their entirety (or ICCs) is rather common for the USA studies. However, the report by Sampson et al. from the Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods (PHDCN) is one exception [107]. In examining the relationship between neighborhood level collective efficacy and violent crime they reported both variance components for within neighborhoods (0.320) and between neighborhoods (0.026) and the ICC was estimated about 7.51%.

In the Canadian context Veenstra also reported both variance components. He found that two measures of health, i.e. the presence of long-term illness and self-rated health status, were predicted by individual-level factors only (i.e. with 0% ICC). The measure of depressive symptoms had some variability, (with 2.1% ICC) that could be reasonably attributed to the community [112].

Studies in western Europe

The studies included in this review did not report random part results.

Studies in Scandinavia

In contrast to the North American studies, area level random parameters have generally been found to be very low (low ICC) in Swedish studies. Lindström and colleagues observed that neighborhood variance in self-rated health was mainly affected by individual factors other than individual social capital (assessed by social participation) with 0.0% ICC [15].

Cross-country studies

The studies included in this review did not report random part results.

Summary of findings

We have reviewed 30 single level studies (using both individual and ecological level data) and 12 multilevel studies from different countries, mainly OECD countries in North America, Europe (both eastern and western) and Australia. It is evident that most of the studies operationalized social capital as a combination of both cognitive (particularly, trust and reciprocity) and structural (informal participation or civic engagement) dimensions of social capital. Moreover, to construct the area level social capital variable most of the studies used the 'aggregated' type of social capital. Out of 42 studies, four studies employed both an 'aggregated' and a contextual social capital measure and four studies (2 single level and 2 multilevel) employed merely contextual social capital in analyzing the health impact of contextual effects.

A summary of results on the associations between social capital and health by different countries is presented in Table 1. Studies using single level analysis found significant relationships between social capital and health. Particularly for the USA virtually all studies found a strong association between lower mortality/better health status and greater stocks of social capital. For Canada, the findings were weaker and less consistent. Except for one Finnish study, significant associations were also observed in both eastern and western Europe including the Scandinavian countries. The findings from Australia were mixed. Two studies reported fairly weak associations between social capital and health. Studies based on cross-country and World Values Survey Data also showed very little or weak and inconsistent associations between social capital and self-rated health or mortality.

For multilevel studies, irrespective of the country's overall degree of material egalitarianism, the fixed-effect results found significant relationships between health and social capital. However, when looking at the studies' random part results one finds that they reported dissimilar results depending on the country's context. In the context of societies with higher degrees of egalitarianism (countries such as Sweden and Canada), multilevel studies report very small ICCs for health outcomes. By contrast, for countries which are not egalitarian, for instance the USA studies reveal higher ICCs, implying greater neighborhood variance in mortality or health. One interpretation is that neighborhoods or areas (characterized by stock of social capital) play a negligible role in explaining health variations in egalitarian countries, even if it was found to be significantly associated with health. In other words, area characteristics such as social capital appear to play a comparatively greater role in less egalitarian societies.

Discussion and conclusions

In this review we have tried to provide an overview of the definitions and forms of social capital and the probable theoretical associations between social capital and health. Recent empirical studies on associations between individual and area level social capital with various health outcomes have also been reviewed systematically. To summarize, it is evident from our review that most of the studies consider some combination of cognitive and structural forms of social capital and operationalize social capital using individual level trust and reciprocity along with informal participation as indicators. Only one study considered the vertical dimension of contextual social capital using Irish data. Although different studies have conceptualized social capital and combined its several components rather differently, irrespective of a country's overall degree of economic egalitarianism, most single level studies have reported a positive association between social capital (both at the individual level and area level) and better health. However, the findings seemed different for studies employing multilevel analysis where the health impacts of 'aggregated'/contextual social capital have been separated from individual compositional effects. Multilevel studies based on US data have generally demonstrated area level fixed and random effects of social capital on population health with higher intra-cluster correlation. By contrast, Swedish and Canadian studies supports an association in the fixed effects, but area level random parameters are generally very low (i.e. low ICC).

Due to the small number of multilevel studies and very few studies reporting random part results, it is difficult to draw any definite conclusions on the basis of our observations. Nonetheless, the proportion of area level variance to total variance (ICC) was quantitatively similar to those presented in the literature emphasizing the significance of area effects. For instance, Fisher et al. examined the variation in self-reported physical activity among older adults in Portland, USA and concluded that social capital (social cohesion) is associated with increased levels of physical activity among older adults with an estimated ICC of 4% [117]. Using British household cross-sectional data, McCulloch tested variations in neighborhood social capital and social disorganization. For social capital he found ICCs of 9.0% for men and 11.3% for women, while for social disorganization the estimated ICCs were even higher, 21.2% for men and 25.5% for women [118]. The Scottish Heart Health Study found that ICC (district level variance of total variance) was between 0.5% (serum cholesterol) and 5.7% (alcohol consumption and smoking) for coronary heart disease risk factors [119].

On the other hand, in a recent study using a four-year follow up study of the entire Swedish population aged 40–64, Sundquist et al. examined whether neighborhood deprivation (another area attribute, an index measured at small area market statistics level by the use of Care Need Index) predicts incidence rates of coronary heart disease (CHD) [120]. They concluded that high levels of neighborhood deprivation independently influence CHD for both men and women, although the ICCs were reported at 0.9 % for men and 2.1% for women. Other Swedish studies also reported similarly low ICCs. For instance, Lindström and colleagues observed that neighborhood variance in daily tobacco smoking was mainly affected by individual factors other than individual social capital (assessed by social participation) and the intra-neighborhood correlation was 1.9% (ICC) [121]. In another study, Lindström et al. concluded that leisure time physical activity is mainly influenced by individual factors, and area level social capital (aggregated) did not explain the neighborhood difference in physical inactivity. They reported the ICC (intra-neighborhood correlation) to be 2.1% [122].

Some caveats may be identified from our review. Firstly, as we have observed, different studies conceptualized social capital differently within the same country and/or across the countries. How robust are these observations considering the fact that different researchers conceptualize social capital differently? In other words, one could raise the question if we are indeed comparing the same phenomenon across the countries? In particular, social participation and trust have been suggested in the literature as representing different aspects of social capital and both these aspects of social capital are thought to mutually enhance each other [123]. However, in reality this is not always the case. According to Fukuyama's notion of 'the miniaturization of community', the more ideologically narrowly defined social networks and organizations destroy generalized trust between people and this phenomenon is observed in the USA [124]. In the Swedish context, Lindström also makes it evident that poor self-reported health is significantly more prevalent in miniaturized (high social participation/low trust) communities compared to high social capital (high social participation/high trust) communities [92]. However, because of limited numbers of studies across countries, the study found difficulties in further disaggregating studies with similar social capital indicators or health outcomes.

The second issue is a corollary of the first; because of small numbers of studies it is difficult to draw definitive conclusions on the basis of our observations. Moreover, social capital may not always generate better health outcomes. Because of the potential importance of social capital externalities (positive and/or negative), the path from individual to aggregate social capital may be difficult to identify [3]. The benefits that social capital creates for one group may disadvantage another, so that the net spill-over effect on society need not be positive [43]. It was also not possible to scrutinize intra-group effect of social capital through our review.

Finally, most of the studies included in our review had a number of methodological limitations. We may exemplify some of the statistical issues that could be regarded as important here. Firstly, for cross-sectional studies (which is the basis of most of the studies), one cannot exclude the possibility of reverse causality. It is recognized that in many contexts, social capital is endogenous and studies have not employed instrumental variables to allow for consistent estimation of parameters. This problem may be expected to be more problematic for individual level social capital studies. In addition, due to imprecision in the indicators of area level social capital executed in different studies, there is the possibility of measurement errors in the explanatory variable and any such errors would lead to inconsistent estimation of models [125]. A general limitation is failure to include other relevant area level predictors in the models. This could mis-specify the fixed part of a multilevel-level statistical model. However, researchers should be cautious about including other contextual predictors and keep in mind the multicolinearity problem. Keeping these methodological limitations in mind, the conclusions observed in some of the reviewed studies need to be interpreted with caution.

Conclusion

It is evident from the fixed effect results across studies (both single level and multilevel) and countries that the positive health impact of social capital and a country's degree of egalitarianism seemed rather unimportant factors in modifying the effects of social capital on health. Secondly, although a significant fixed effect association was observed between area level social capital and better health in multilevel studies, low variability in health across areas reported in some studies suggests that the differences in health were predominantly affected by individual factors rather than by area characteristics, especially in egalitarian countries.

We have raised the question whether egalitarianism matters in mediating the health impact of social capital and have tried to find some answers through reviewing existing literature. However, many questions still remain unanswered about the notion of social capital and interrelations between social capital and health. Before making further definitive conclusions that social capital is an important determinant of population health (or health differences) or that the egalitarian distribution of income and wealth matters for conditioning the health impact or differences of social capital, careful exploration is warranted through employing appropriate statistical methods (e.g. multilevel analysis) and data. It is expected that the observations from this review will have implications for the content and direction of future research concerning social capital and social determinants of health, on policy formation as well as on the implementation of health service delivery.

References

  1. Durkheim E: Suicide: a study in sociology. Edited by: Simpson G, Spaulding JA, Simpson G 1951. 1897, New York: Free Press

    Google Scholar 

  2. Dasgupta P: Economic Progress and the Idea of Social Capital, in Social Capital: A Multifaceted Perspectives. Edited by: Dasgupta P, Seragilden I. 2000, Wasington DC: World Bank

    Google Scholar 

  3. Glaeser EL, Laibson D, Sacerdote B: An Economic Approach to Social Capital. The Economic Journal. 2002, 112 (November): F437-F458. 10.1111/1468-0297.00078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Knack S, Keefer P: Does social capital have an economic pay-off? A cross-country investigation. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 1997, 112: 1251-1288. 10.1162/003355300555475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Durlauf S: On the Empirics of Social Capital. Economic Journal. 2002, 112 (483): 459-479. 10.1111/1468-0297.00079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Kawachi I, Berkman L: Social cohesion, social capital and health. Social Epidemiology. Edited by: Barkman LF, Kawachi I. 2000, New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  7. Kawachi I, Kennedy B, Glass R: Social capital and self-rated health: A contextual analysis. American Journal of Public Health. 1999, 89 (8): 1187-1193.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Kennedy B, Kawachi I, Brainerd E: The role of social capital in the Russian mortality crisis. World Development. 1998, 26 (11): 2029-2043. 10.1016/S0305-750X(98)00094-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Lochner K, Kawachi I, Kennedy BP: Social capital: a guide to its measurement. Health & Place. 1999, 5: 259-270. 10.1016/S1353-8292(99)00016-7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Kawachi I, Kim D, Coutts A, Subramanian SV: Reconciling the three accounts of social capital. Int J Epidemiol. 2004, 33: 682-690. 10.1093/ije/dyh177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Coleman JS: Foundations of Social Theory. 1990, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  12. Putnam RD: Bowling alone: the collapse and revival of American community. 2000, New York: Simon and Schuster

    Book  Google Scholar 

  13. Subramanian SV, Lochner K, Kawachi I: Neighborhood differences in social capital: a compositional artifact or a contextual construct?. Health & Place. 2003, 9: 33-44. 10.1016/S1353-8292(02)00028-X.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Subramanian SV, Kim DJ, Kawachi I: Social Trust and Self-Rated Health in US Communities: A Multilevel Analysis. J of Urban Health: Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine. 2002, 79 (4): S21-S34. 10.1093/jurban/79.suppl_1.S21.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Lindström M, Moghaddassi M, Merlo J: Individual self-reported health, social participation and neighborhood: a multilevel analysis in Malmö, Sweden. Preventive Medicine. 2004, 39: 135-141. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.01.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Merlo J, Lynch JW, Yang M, Lindstrom M, Ostergren PO, Rasmusen NK, RÃ¥stam L: Effect of neighborhood social participation on individual use of hormone replacement therapy and antihypertensive medication: a multilevel analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 2003, 157 (9): 774-783. 10.1093/aje/kwg053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Kawachi I, Kennedy B, Lochner K, Prothrow-Stith D: Social Capital, Income Inequality, and Mortality. American Journal of Public Health. 1997, 87 (9): 1491-1498.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Bolin K, Lindgren B, Lindström M, Nystedt P: Investments in social capital – implications of social interactions for the production of health. Social Science and Medicine. 2003, 56 (12): 2379-2390. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00242-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Arnesen Richard: Egalitarianism. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Edited by: Edward N. Zalta. 2002, [http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2002/entries/egalitarianism]Fall 2002

    Google Scholar 

  20. Mahler V, Jesuit D: Fiscal distribution in the developed countries: new insights from the Luxembourg Income Study. Luxembourg Income Study Working Paper, September 2005 (Forthcoming Socio-Economic Review, 2006)

  21. Förster M, Mira d'Ercole M: Income distribution and poverty in OECD countries in the selected half of the 1990s. DELSA/ELSA/WD/SEM (2005)1. 2005, OECD Social, Employment and Migration Working Paper No. 22

    Google Scholar 

  22. OECD: Social expenditure data base . 2004, [http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/56/37/31613113.xls]

    Google Scholar 

  23. McAule A: Social Policy and the Labor Market in Russia During Transition. Transforming post-communist political economies. Task Force on Economies in Transition, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Edited by: nelson et al. 1998, National Research Council, National Academy Press Washington, D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Kumlin S, Rothstein B: Making and breaking social capital -The impact of welfare-state institutions. Comparative Political Studies. 2005, 38 (4): 339-365. 10.1177/0010414004273203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Wolfe A: Whose keeper? Social science and moral obligation. 1989, Berkeley: University of California Press

    Google Scholar 

  26. Rothstein B: Social Capital in Social Democratic State. The Swedish Model and Civil Society. Politics and Society. 2001, 29 (2): 209-240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Rothstein B, Stolle D: Introduction: Social Capital in Scandinavia. Scandinavian Political Studies. 2003, 26 (1): 1-26. 10.1111/1467-9477.t01-1-00077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Kawachi I: Long Live Community: Social Capital as Public Health. The American Prospect. 1997, November/December: 56-59.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Wilkinson RG: Socioeconomic determinants of health: Health inequalities: relative or absolute material standards?. BMJ. 1997, 314 (7080): 591-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Wilkinson RG: Unhealthy societies: the afflictions of inequality. 1996, London: Routledge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  31. Ross NA, Wolfson MC, Dunn JR, Berthelot J-M, Kaplan GA, Lynch JW: Relation between income inequality and mortality in Canada and in the United States: cross sectional assessment using census data and vital statistics. British Medical Journal. 2000, 320: 898-902.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Gerdtham U-G, Johannesson M: Absolute Income, Relative Income, Income Inequality and Mortality. Journal of Human Resources. 2004, 39 (1): 228-247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Portes A: Social capital: its origins and applications in modern sociology. Annual Review of Sociology. 1998, 24: 1-24. 10.1146/annurev.soc.24.1.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Woolcock M: Social Capital and Economic Development: Towards a Theoretical Synthesis and Policy Framework. Theory and Society. 1998, 2 (27): 151-208. 10.1023/A:1006884930135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Macinko J, Starfield B: The utility of social capital in research on health determinants. The Milbank Quarterly. 2001, 79: 387-427. 10.1111/1468-0009.00213.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Bourdieu P: Le capital social: Notes provisoires. Actes Recherche Science Social. 1980, 31: 2-3.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Bourdieu P: The forms of capital. Handbook of Theory and Research for the Sociology of Education. Edited by: Richardson JG. 1986, New York: Greenwood, 241-258.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Swedberg R: Economic sociology: past and present. Current Sociology. 1987, 35 (1): 1-215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Hanifan LJ: The Community Center. 1920, Boston: Silver, Burdett

    Google Scholar 

  40. Hanifan LJ: The rural school community center. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 1916, 67: 130-138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Putnam RD: Bowling alone: America's Declining Social Capital. Journal of Democracy. 1995, 6 (1): 65-78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Jacobs J: The Death and Life of Great American Cities. New York: Vintage. 1961

    Google Scholar 

  43. Durlauf S, Fafchamps M: Social Capital. National Bureau of Economic Research (NBER) Working Paper 10485 National Bureau of Economic Research 1050 Massachusetts Avenue Cambridge, MA 02138. 2004, [http://www.nber.org/papers/w10485]

    Google Scholar 

  44. Loury G: A Dynamic Theory of Radical Income Differences. Women, Minorities and Employment Discrimination. Edited by: Wallace P, LeMund A. 1977, Lexington: Lexington Books

    Google Scholar 

  45. Putnam RD, Leonardi R, Nanetti R: Making Democracy Work: Civic Tradition in Modern Italy. 1993, Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  46. Krishna A, Uphoff N: Mapping and Measuring Social Capital: A Conceptual and Empirical Study of Collective Action for Conserving and Developing Watersheds in Rajastan, India. Social Capital Initiative Working Paper no. 13. 1999, Washington D.C., World Bank

    Google Scholar 

  47. McKenzie K, Whitley R, Weich S: Social Capital and mental health. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2002, 181: 280-283. 10.1192/bjp.181.4.280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Harpham T, Grant E, Thomas E: Measuring social capital within health surveys: key issues. Health Policy and Planning. 2002, 17 (1): 106-111. 10.1093/heapol/17.1.106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Cullen M, Whiteford H: The Interrelations of Social Capital with Health and Mental Health. Discussion Paper. 2001, The Commonwealth of Australia

    Google Scholar 

  50. Szreter S, Woolcock M: Health by association? Social capital, social theory, and the political economy of public health. Int J Epidemiol. 2004, 33: 650-667. 10.1093/ije/dyh013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Grootaert C, Narayan D, Jones VN, Woolcock M: Measuring Social Capital: An Integrated Questionnaire. Working Paper #18. 2004, The World Bank. Washington, D.C

    Google Scholar 

  52. Gittell R, Vidal A: Community Organizing: Building Social Capital as a Development Strategy. 1998, Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications

    Google Scholar 

  53. Narayan D: Bonds and Bridges: Social Capital and Poverty. Social Capital and Economic Development: Well-Being in Developing Countries. Edited by: Jonathan Isham, Thomas Kelly, Sunder Ramaswamy. 2002, Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar, 58-81.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Mitchell CU, LaGory M: Social capital and mental distress in an impoverished community. City & Community. 2002, 1 (2): 195-215. 10.1111/1540-6040.00017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Varshney A: Ethnic Conflict and Civic Life. Hindus and Muslims in India. 2002, Yale University Press

    Google Scholar 

  56. Baum F: Social Capital: Is It Good for Your Health? Issues for a Public Health Agenda. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1999, 53 (1): 195-196.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Gillies P: Social Capital: recognising the value of society. Healthiness. 1997, September: 15-17.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Campbell C: Social Capital and Health: Contextualizing Health Promotion with Local Community Networks. Social Capital: Critical Perspectives. Edited by: Stephen Baron, John Field, Tom Schuller. 2000, Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  59. Durlauf S: Bowling Alone: A Review Essay. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization. 2000, 47 (3): 259-273. 10.1016/S0167-2681(01)00210-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Manaski CF: Economic Analysis of social interactions. Journal of Economic Perspectives. 2000, 14: 115-136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Fukuyama F: Social capital and civil society. International Monetary Fund Working Paper, 74. 2000, International Monetary Fund, Washington DC

    Google Scholar 

  62. Tunstall HVZ, Shaw M, Dorling D: Places and Health. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004, 58 (1): 6-10. 10.1136/jech.58.1.6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Openshaw S, Taylor P: A million or so correlation coefficients: Three experiments on the modifiable area unit problem. Statistical applications in the Spatial Sciences. Edited by: Wrigley N. 1979, London: Pion, 127-144.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Heywood : Introduction to Geographical Information Systems. 1998, New York: Addison Wesley Longman

    Google Scholar 

  65. Nakaya T: An information statistical approach to the modifiable areal unit problem in incidence rate maps. Environment & Planning A. 2000, 32 (1): 91-109. 10.1068/a31145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Chaix B, Merlo J, Subramanian SV, Lynch J, Chauvin P: Comparison of a spatial perspective with the multilevel analytical approach in neighborhood studies: the case of mental and behavioral disorders due to psychoactive substance use in Malmo, Sweden, 2001. Am J Epidemiol. 2005, 162 (2): 171-182. 10.1093/aje/kwi175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Muntaner C, Lynch J, Davey Smith G: Social capital, disorganized communities, and the third way: understanding the retreat from structural inequalities in epidemiology and public health. International Journal of Health Services. 2001, 31: 213-237. 10.2190/NVW3-4HH0-74PX-AC38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Lundborg P: Having the wrong friends? Peer effects in adolescent binge drinking, smoking and illicit-drug use. Risky Health Behavior Among Adolescent. Lund Economic Studies 10. 2003, Department of Economics, Lund University, Sweden

    Google Scholar 

  69. Muntaner C, Lynch J: Income inequality, social cohesion, and class relations: A critique of Wilkinson's neo-Durkheimian research program. International Journal of Health Services. 1999, 29 (1): 59-81. 10.2190/G8QW-TT09-67PL-QTNC.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Pearce N, Davey Smith G: Is social capital the key to inequalities in health?. Am J Public Health. 2003, 93 (1): 122-129.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Navarro V: A critique of social capital. Int J Health Serv. 2002, 32 (4): 424-432.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Schneider MC, Castillo-Salgado C, Bacallao J, Loyola E, Mujica OJ, Vidaurre M, Roca A: Methods for measuring health inequalities (Part I). Epidemiological Bulletin. 2004, 25 (4):

  73. Diez-Roux AV: A glossary for multilevel analysis. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2002, 56: 588-594. 10.1136/jech.56.8.588.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Wilkinson RG, Kawachi I, Kennedy BP: Mortality, the social environment, crime and violence. Social Health Illness. 1998, 20: 578-597. 10.1111/1467-9566.00120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Lochner K, Kawachi I, Brennan RT, Buka SL: Social capital and neighborhood mortality rates in Chicago. Social Science and Medicine. 2003, 56: 1797-1805. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00177-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Holtgrave DR, Crosby RA: Social capital poverty, and income inequality as predictors of gonorrhoea, syphilis, Chlamydia and AIDS in the United States. Sex Transm Infect. 2003, 79: 62-64. 10.1136/sti.79.1.62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Milyo J, Mellor JM: On the importance of age-adjustment methods in ecological studies of social determinants of mortality. Health Serv Res. 2003, 38 (6): 1781-1790. 10.1111/j.1475-6773.2003.00202.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Holtgrave DR, Crosby RA: Social determinants of tuberculosis case rates in the United States. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2004, 26 (2): 159-162. 10.1016/j.amepre.2003.10.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Veenstra G: Social capital, SES and health: an individual-level analysis. Social Science and Medicine. 2000, 50: 619-629. 10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00307-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Veenstra G: Social capital and health (plus wealth, income inequality and region health governance). Soc Sci Med. 2002, 54: 849-868. 10.1016/S0277-9536(01)00049-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Veenstra G, Luginaah I, Wakefield S, Birch S, Eyles J, Elliott S: Who you know, where you live: social capital, neighborhood and health. Soc Sci Med. 2005, 60 (12): 2799-2818. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Rose R: How much does social capital add to individual health? A survey of Russians. Soc Sci Med. 2000, 51: 1421-1435. 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00106-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Skrabski A, Kopp M, Kawachi I: Social capital in a changing society: cross sectional associations with middle aged female and male mortality. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003, 57: 114-119. 10.1136/jech.57.2.114.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Skrabski A, Kopp M, Kawachi I: ocial capital and collective efficacy in Hungray: cross sectional associations with middle aged female and male mortality rates. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004, 58: 340-345. 10.1136/jech.2003.010017.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. McCulloch A: Social environments and health: cross sectional national survey. British Medical Journal. 2001, 323: 208-209.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Kelleher C, Timoney A, Friel S, McKeown D: Indicators of deprivation, voting patterns, and health status at area level in the Republic of Ireland. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2002, 56 (1): 36-44. 10.1136/jech.56.1.36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Hyppä MT, Mäki J: Why do Swedish-speaking Finns have longer active life? An area of social capital research. Health Promotion International. 2001, 16 (1): 55-64. 10.1093/heapro/16.1.55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Hyppä MT, Mäki J: Individual-level relationships between social capital and self-rated health in a bilingual community. Preventive Medicine. 2001, 32: 148-155. 10.1006/pmed.2000.0782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Hyppä MT, Mäki J: Social participation and health in a community rich in stock of social capital. Health Education Research. 2003, 18 (6): 770-779. 10.1093/her/cyf044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Liukkonen V, Virtanen P, Kivimaki M, Pentti J, Vahtera J: Social capital in working life and the health of employees. Soc Sci Med. 2004, 59 (12): 2447-2458. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.04.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Sundquist K, Lindström M, Malmström M, Johansson S, Sundquist J: Social participation and coronary heart disease: a follow-up study of 6900 women and men in Sweden. Soc Sci Med. 2004, 58: 615-622. 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00229-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Lindström M: Social capital, the miniaturisation of community and self-reported global and psychological health. Soc Sci Med. 2004, 59 (3): 595-605. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.11.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Siahpush M, Singh GK: Social integration and mortality in Australia. Aust N Z J Public Health. 1999, 23 (6): 571-577.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Chavez R, Kemp L, Harris E: The social capital health relationship in two disadvantaged neighborhoods. J Health Serv Res Policy. 2004, 9 (Suppl 2): 29-34. 10.1258/1355819042349871.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Ziersch AM, Baum FE, Macdougall C, Putland C: Neighborhood life and social capital: the implications for health. Soc Sci Med. 2005, 60 (1): 71-86. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.04.027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Lynch JW, Davey Smith G, Hillemeier M, Shaw M, Raghunathan T, Kaplan GA: Income inequality, psychological environment and health: comparisons across wealthy nations. The Lancet. 2001, 358 (9277): 194-200. 10.1016/S0140-6736(01)05407-1.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  97. Kennelly B, O'Shea E, Gavey E: Social Capital, life expectancy and mortality: a cross-national examination. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 56 (12): 2367-2377. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00241-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Smith P, Polanyi M: Social norms, social behaviors and health: an empirical examination of a model of social capital. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2003, 27 (4): 456-463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Carlson P: The European health divide: a matter of financial or social capital?. Soc Sci Med. 2004, 59 (9): 1985-1992. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.03.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Helliwell JF, Putnam RD: The social context of well-being. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B. 2004, 359: 1435-1446. 10.1098/rstb.2004.1522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Pollack CE, von dem Knesebeck O: Social capital and health among the aged: comparisons between the United States and Germany. Health Place. 2004, 10 (4): 383-391. 10.1016/j.healthplace.2004.08.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Goldstein H: Multilevel Statistical Models. 2003, Hodder Arnold, London, 3

    Google Scholar 

  103. Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ: Multilevel analysis: an introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. 1999, Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publications

    Google Scholar 

  104. Merlo J: Multilevel analytical approaches in social epidemiology: measures of health variation compared with traditional measures of association. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003, 57 (8): 550-552. 10.1136/jech.57.8.550.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  105. Diez-Roux AV: Multilevel analysis in public health research. Annu Rev Public Health. 2000, 21: 171-192. 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.21.1.171.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Bingenheimer JB, Raudenbush SW: Statistical and substantive inferences in public health: issues in the application of multilevel models. Annu Rev Public Health. 2004, 25: 53-77. 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.25.050503.153925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F: Neighborhoods and violent crime: a multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science. 1997, 277: 918-924. 10.1126/science.277.5328.918.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Subramanian SV, Kawachi I, Kennedy BP: Does the state you live in make a difference? Multilevel analysis of self-rated health in the US. Soc Sci Med. 2001, 53: 9-19. 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00309-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Browning CR, Cagney KA: Collective efficacy and health: neighborhood social capital and self-rated physical functioning in an urban setting. J Health Soc Behav. 2002, 43: 383-399. 10.2307/3090233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Wen M, Browning CR, Cagney KA: Poverty affluence and income inequality: neighborhood economic structure and its implications for health. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 57: 843-860. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00457-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Franzini L, Spears W: Contributions of social context to inequalities in years of life lost to heart disease in Texas, USA. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 57 (10): 1847-1861. 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00018-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Veenstra G: Location, location, location: contextual and compositional health effects of social capital in British Columbia, Canada. Soc Sci Med. 2005, 60 (9): 2059-2071. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.08.064.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Drukker M, Kaplan C, Feron F, Os van J: Children's health-related quality of life, neighborhood socioeconomic deprivation and social capital. A contextual analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 7: 825-841. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00453-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Mohan J, Twigg L, Barnard S, Jones K: Social capital, geography and health: a small- area analysis for England. Soc Sci Med. 2005, 60: 1267-1283. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.050.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Drukker M, Buka SL, Kaplan C, McKenzie K, Van Os J: Social capital and young adolescents' perceived health in different sociocultural settings. Soc Sci Med. 2005, 61 (1): 185-198. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Rice N, Jones AM: Multilevel models and health economics. Health Economics. 1997, 6: 562-575. 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1050(199711)6:6<561::AID-HEC288>3.0.CO;2-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  117. Fisher KJ, Li F, Michael Y, Cleveland M: Neighborhood-level influences on physical activity among older adults: a multilevel analysis. J Aging and Physical Activity. 2004, 11: 45-63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  118. McCulloch A: An examination of social capital and social disorganization in neighborhood in the British household panel study. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 56: 1425-1438. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00139-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Hart C, Ecob R, Smith GD: People, places and coronary heart disease risk factors: a multilevel analysis of the Scottish Heart Health Study archive. Soc Sci Med. 1997, 45 (6): 893-902. 10.1016/S0277-9536(96)00431-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Sundquist K, Malmstrom M, Johansson SE: Neighborhood deprivation and incidence of coronary heart disease: a multilevel study of 2.6 million women and men in Sweden. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004, 58: 71-77. 10.1136/jech.58.1.71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  121. Lindström M, Moghaddassi M, Bolin K, Lindgren B, Merlo J: Social participation, social capital and daily tobacco smoking: a population-based multilevel analysis in Malmo, Sweden. Scand J Public Health. 2003, 31 (6): 444-450. 10.1080/14034940310006203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  122. Lindström M, Moghaddassi M, Merlo J: Social capital leisure-time physical activity: a population based multilevel analysis in Malmö, Sweden. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003, 57 (1): 23-28. 10.1136/jech.57.1.23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  123. Putnam RD: The Prosperous Community: Social Capital and Economic Growth. American Prospect. 1993, 35-42. Spring

  124. Fukuyama F: The great disruption. Human nature and the reconstitution of social order. London: Profile Books. 1999

    Google Scholar 

  125. Wooldridge JM: Introductory Econometrics: A Modern Approach. 2000, South-Western Modern Publishing. Thomson Learning

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank three anonymous referees for helpful comments and suggestions. Financial support from the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (Ulf-G Gerdtham, #2002-0376, Juan Merlo #2003-580), the Swedish Research Council (Juan Merlo, #2004-6155) and the Swedish National Institute of Public Health (Ulf-G Gerdtham) is gratefully acknowledged. Kawachi is partly supported by the MacArthur Network on SES and Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to M Kamrul Islam.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

M. K. Islam, U-G. Gerdtham and J. Merlo conceived the study. M. K. Islam, U-G. Gerdtham, J. Merlo, I. Kawachi and M. Lindström designed the study. M. K. Islam analyzed and wrote the paper. All authors provided significant comments, wrote, read, and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Islam, M.K., Merlo, J., Kawachi, I. et al. Social capital and health: Does egalitarianism matter? A literature review. Int J Equity Health 5, 3 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-9276-5-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-9276-5-3

Keywords